Teaching new teachers all the wrong things

Embed from Getty Images

There is a classic research design in education. Instead of investigating the effect of some intervention or other on children in schools, researchers study student teachers. The researchers typically try to change the attitudes of the students teachers towards some aspect of teaching and usually in a counterproductive way.

This design arises repeatedly because student teachers are easier to study than schoolchildren – they are available in the universities where researchers work and the ethics requirements tend to be less demanding – and because of a critical point about all research of this kind. First, the researcher assumes a position, then the researcher seeks to influence the student teachers to adopt this position, something that is relatively easy to do given that the student teachers want to pass their course. All the while, the position itself is never questioned. There is no chance of generating inconvenient data that may challenge the position.

Barry Garelick pointed me to a good example in the US version of The Conversation.

In this case, the student teachers are learning how to teach science and maths. Apparently, these student teachers tend to tell the researcher – Peter Cormas, an Associate Professor in science education – that children, “must memorize science and mathematics knowledge to learn it.” Instead, the professor wants them to accept that children can, “acquire knowledge through a process used by scientists and mathematicians called problem-solving.”

Replace ‘memorize’ with ‘remember’ and the concept that children must remember science and mathematics knowledge to learn it seems perfectly sensible. Moreover, in the alternative approach, do children not remember what they acquire through problem-solving? I admit this is possible, but it does then draw into question the efficacy of such an approach.

And there are good reasons to question this efficacy. Although Cormas seems unaware of this, there’s quite a debate about the value of discovery learning when compared to explicitly teaching the same content. Many advocates of problem-based learning are aware of this debate and tend to emphasise the amount of guidance they give students. Not so, Cormas, who is quite happy to advocate for discovery:

“Throughout the course, I asked the future teachers to discover science and mathematics knowledge with problem-solving. I also had the future teachers teach students at a local school by asking them to learn with problem-solving.”

Regular readers of this blog are probably already familiar with the research in this area, but here is a whistle-stop tour: In this article, Mayer makes a convincing case against pure discovery learning drawing upon historical examples, in this seminal work, Kirschner, Sweller and Clark survey the empirical evidence on approaches based on problem-solving, and in this piece, Kirschner blows apart the commonly-held notion that the best way for novices to learn a discipline is by imitating the behaviour of professionals.

And regular readers will also be aware of the explanation that Cognitive Load Theory provides for these findings: Discovery learning overwhelms working memory for novices who possess little to draw upon from long-term memory.

However, Cormas never seems to question his questionable assumption that learning by problem-solving is superior. Instead, he simply asserts, without references, that, “because problem-solving is necessary for scientific and mathematical literacy, students need teachers who will expose them to problem-solving.”

And Cormas lacks a little curiosity when he notes that, “former students that I ran into years later often told me that they do not use problem-solving as teachers. Instead, they reverted to simply asking students to memorize science and mathematics information. They told me the reason for this is that teachers in their present schools do not use problem-solving. I find this troubling.”

Is it worth considering – I mean, just for a moment – whether the experienced professionals who teach science and maths every day may be avoiding problem-solving for a reason? Is their craft knowledge worth exploring? At all?

The reason the grizzled old teachers are misbehaving, of course, is that learning through problem-solving is, at best, extremely inefficient and, at worst, does not work. One of the peculiarities of our species is that we have evolved the means to communicate complex ideas to each other. This has enabled us to pick-up where a previous generation left-off and develop ever more sophisticated iterations of science and mathematics. It seems perverse to cast this aside and insist that children figure out science and mathematics for themselves.

Research of this kind will continue to take place in universities. I see no prospect of that changing. So, as a profession, we need to seek new ways to disrupt and bypass these institutions that teach our newest members all the wrong things. As teachers, we need to be at the centre of that disruption, taking charge of our profession for ourselves.

Advertisement
Standard

2 thoughts on “Teaching new teachers all the wrong things

  1. The Next Generation Science Standards are a Trojan Horse filled with all of the essential ingredients of a Common Core approach to teaching K to 12 science with a decided emphasis on discovery learning and project-based problem solving. Achieve is the corporate think tank behind the creation of the NGSS, leveraging their push via the worn out straw-man argument that “the memorization of disconnected facts” is the bane of science instruction. Treating very young, novice learners as if they are equally equipped to solve problems as adult scientists is wrong on every level. Discovery learning is slow, frustrating, ineffective, unfair, and a major turn-off for students who desperately need a sage on the stage – not a guide on the side. The new twist coming from NGSS is the bashing of teaching science in “silos” (traditional disciplines) while pushing integrated instruction at each grade level. Abandoning the traditional scope and sequence is being foisted on teachers with absolutely no evidence to support the claims of superiority. Instead, we get the conflation of how professional scientists conduct their work with the best ways for children to learn the basics. There is enough mumbo-jumbo and edu-jargon in the NGSS Trojan Horse to fool administrators, BOEs, and parents into thinking that the long debunked discovery method and the “one inch wide, one mile deep” approach is a new, shiny silver bullet that will magically transform children into mini-adults.

  2. George Polya, the Stanford math professor authored a book called “How to Solve It”. It was a book written for an audience of upper grade high school students and college students who had some degree of expertise in math. It has largely been misinterpreted as a guide to teaching how to solve problems. Problem solving is something that cannot be taught in isolation. It is done in tandem with domain specific knowledge; there is no general “problem solving” schema. (See http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.849.5087&rep=rep1&type=pdf )

    Polya himself has also said: “Solving problems is a practical art, like swimming, or skiing, or playing the piano: you can learn it only by imitation and practice.”

Leave a Reply

Fill in your details below or click an icon to log in:

WordPress.com Logo

You are commenting using your WordPress.com account. Log Out /  Change )

Facebook photo

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Log Out /  Change )

Connecting to %s

This site uses Akismet to reduce spam. Learn how your comment data is processed.